Kan een tekst te simpel zijn? Hoe lager en hoger opgeleiden oordelen over eenvoudige taal | Amsterdam University Press Journals Online
2004
Volume 44, Issue 2
  • ISSN: 1573-9775
  • E-ISSN: 2352-1236

Abstract

Abstract

Text consultants often encounter the fear that simple language comes across as childish, especially for higher educated readers. We sought to empirically test this assumption by presenting participants with two letters from organizations (a bank and a residential care facility) to their customers. Both lexical and syntactic complexity were varied in the two texts.

In the first text, simple words both improve actual comprehension and perceived comprehensibility. The author who uses simple words is also seen as more friendly, an effect mediated by the comprehensibility judgment. In the second text, simple words again improve actual comprehension, but only in the syntactically complex text versions. Most strikingly, only the higher educated readers preferred the simpler text versions over the more complex ones.

Overall, there is no reason to fear that simple language offends readers in any way. It is less clear why the results differ for our two texts; different language expectations regarding banks and care facilities may play a role. A more fundamental question is how exactly simple language succeeds in generating positive attitudes towards the author; we end by outlining various possible explanations.

Loading

Article metrics loading...

/content/journals/10.5117/TVT2022.2.002.MAAT
2022-12-01
2024-03-29
Loading full text...

Full text loading...

/deliver/fulltext/15739775/44/2/TVT2022.2.002.MAAT.html?itemId=/content/journals/10.5117/TVT2022.2.002.MAAT&mimeType=html&fmt=ahah

References

  1. Averbeck, J. M., & Miller, C. (2014). Expanding language expectancy theory: The suasory effects of lexical complexity and syntactic complexity on effective message design. Communication Studies, 65(1), 72-95. doi: 10.1080/10510974.2013.775955.
    [Google Scholar]
  2. Bunt, M. (2016, 1december). Jip-en-Janneke-taal is voor kleuters. Schrijf volwassen!Buntblog. https://www.linkedin.com/pulse/jip-en-janneke-taal-voor-kleuters-schrijf-volwassen-maarten-bunt/?originalSubdomain=nl
    [Google Scholar]
  3. DePaulo, B. M., & Coleman, L. M. (1987). Verbal and nonverbal communication of warmth to children, foreigners, and retarded adults. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 11(2), 75-88. doi: 10.1007/BF00990959.
    [Google Scholar]
  4. Diversiteitspraktijk (z.d.). Hoe spreek ik in heldere taal?https://www.diversiteitspraktijk.be/artikels/hoe-spreek-ik-in-heldere-taal
  5. Duffy, T.M., & Kabance, P. (1982). Testing a readable writing approach to text revision. Journal of Educational Psychology, 74(5), 733-748. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.74.5.733.
    [Google Scholar]
  6. Freebody, P., & Anderson, R. C. (1983a). Effects of vocabulary difficulty, text cohesion, and schema availability on reading comprehension. Reading Research Quarterly, 18(3), 277-294. doi:
    [Google Scholar]
  7. Freebody, P., & Anderson, R. C. (1983b). Effects on text comprehension of differing proportions and locations of difficult vocabulary. Journal of Reading Behavior, 15(3), 19-39. doi: 10.1080/10862968309547487.
    [Google Scholar]
  8. Gasiorek, J. (2016). The “dark side” of CAT: Nonaccommodation. In H.Giles (Ed.), Communication accommodation theory: Negotiating personal relationships and social identities across contexts (pp. 13–35). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  9. Gasiorek, J., & Giles, H. (2012). Effects of inferred motive on evaluations of nonaccommodative communication. Human Communication Research, 38(3), 309-331. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-2958.2012.01426.x.
    [Google Scholar]
  10. Gasiorek, J., & Giles, H. (2015). The role of inferred motive in processing nonaccommodation: Evaluations of communication and speakers. Western Journal of Communication, 79(4), 456-471. doi: 10.1080/10570314.2015.1066030.
    [Google Scholar]
  11. Grice, P. (1989). Studies in the Way of Words. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  12. Hehman, J. A., & Bugental, D. B. (2015). Responses to patronizing communication and factors that attenuate those responses. Psychology and Aging, 30(3), 552. doi: 10.1037/pag0000041.
    [Google Scholar]
  13. Heij, K., & Visser, W. (2006). Schrijven in eenvoudig Nederlands. Sdu Uitgevers.
    [Google Scholar]
  14. Heylighen, F., & Dewaele, J. M. (2002). Variation in the contextuality of language: An empirical measure. Foundations of science, 7(3), 293-340. doi: 10.1023/A:1019661126744.
    [Google Scholar]
  15. Hovy, E. (1987). Generating natural language under pragmatic constraints. Journal of Pragmatics, 11(6), 689-719. doi: 10.1016/0378-2166(87)90109-3.
    [Google Scholar]
  16. Huis van het Nederlands (z.d.). Checklist duidelijk geschreven taal. http://www.klaretaalrendeert.be/files/Checklist%20duidelijk%20geschreven%20taal.pdf
  17. Kemper, S., & Harden, T. (1999). Experimentally disentangling what’s beneficial about elderspeak from what’s not. Psychology and Aging, 14(4), 656-670. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.14.4.656.
    [Google Scholar]
  18. Kemper, S., Othick, M., Gerhing, H., Gubarchuk, J., & Billington, C. (1998). The effects of practicing speech accommodations to older adults. Applied Psycholinguistics, 19(2), 175-192. doi: 10.1017/s014271640001002x.
    [Google Scholar]
  19. Kleijn, S. (2018). Clozing in on readability. How linguistic features affect and predict text comprehension and on-line processing. Proefschrift Universiteit Utrecht. https://dspace.library.uu.nl/handle/1874/363346
    [Google Scholar]
  20. Jensen, M. L., Averbeck, J. M., Zhang, Z., & Wright, K. B. (2013). Credibility of anonymous online product reviews: A language expectancy perspective. Journal of Management Information Systems, 30(1), 293-324.
    [Google Scholar]
  21. Norman, S., Kemper, S., & Kynette, D. (1992). Adults’reading comprehension: Effects of syntactic complexity and working memory. Journal of Gerontology, 47(4), 258-265. doi: 10.1093/geronj/47.4.P258.
    [Google Scholar]
  22. Okuhara, T., Ishikawa, H., Okada, M., Kato, M., & Kiuchi, T. (2017). Designing persuasive health materials using processing fluency: a literature review. BMC Research Notes, 10(1), p. 198. doi: 10.1186/s13104-017-2524-x.
    [Google Scholar]
  23. Okuhara, T., Ishikawa, H., Ueno, H., Okada, H., Kato, M., & Kiuchi, T. (2020). Influence of high versus low readability level of written health information on self-efficacy: A randomized controlled study of the processing fluency effect. Health Psychology Open, 7(1) doi: 10.1177/2055102920905627.
    [Google Scholar]
  24. Oppenheimer, D. M. (2006). Consequences of erudite vernacular utilized irrespective of necessity. Problems with using long words needlessly. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 20(2), 139-156. doi: 10.1002/acp.1178.
    [Google Scholar]
  25. Pander Maat, H., Kraf, R., Bosch, A. van den, Dekker, N., Gompel, M. van, Kleijn, S., Sanders, T.J.M. & Sloot, K. van der (2014). T-Scan: a new tool for analyzing Dutch text. Computational Linguistics in the Netherlands Journal, 4, 53-74.
    [Google Scholar]
  26. Pander Maat, H., & Dekker, N. (2016). Tekstgenres analyseren op lexicale complexiteit met TScan. Tijdschrift voor Taalbeheersing, 38(3), 263-304. doi: 10.5117/tvt2016.3.pand.
    [Google Scholar]
  27. Pander Maat, H. (2017). Zinslengte en zinscomplexiteit: Een corpusbenadering. Tijdschrift voor Taalbeheersing, 39(3), 297-328. doi: /10.5117/tvt2017.3.pand.
    [Google Scholar]
  28. Pander Maat, H., & Ditewig, S. (2017). Hoe worden onderwijsteksten vereenvoudigd, en helpt dat?. Tijdschrift voor Taalbeheersing, 39(2), 245-263. 10.5117/TVT2017.2.MAAT.
    [Google Scholar]
  29. Pander Maat, H. & Van der Geest, T. (2021). Monitor begrijpelijke overheidsteksten. https://www.rijksoverheid.nl/documenten/rapporten/2021/06/30/monitor-begrijpelijke-overheidsteksten
    [Google Scholar]
  30. Partners in tekst (z.d.). Duidelijke taal: maak uw tekst begrijpelijk. https://www.overtuigendeteksten.nl/eenvoudige-taal
  31. Rothermich, K., Harris, H. L., Sewell, K., & Bobb, S. C. (2019). Listener impressions of foreigner-directed speech: A systematic review. Speech Communication, 112, 22-29. doi: 10.1016/j.specom.2019.07.002.
    [Google Scholar]
  32. Schwarz, N., Jalbert, M., Noah, T., & Zhang, L. (2021). Metacognitive experiences as information: Processing fluency in consumer judgment and decision making. Consumer Psychology Review, 4(1), 4-25. doi: 10.1002/arcp.1067.
    [Google Scholar]
  33. Shaw, C. A. & Gordon, J. K. (2021). Understanding Elderspeak: An Evolutionary Concept Analysis. Innovation in Aging5 (3), 1-18. doi: 10.1093/geroni/igab023.
    [Google Scholar]
  34. Song, H., & Schwarz, N. (2010). If it’s easy to read, it’s easy to do, pretty, good, and true. Psychologist, 23(2), 108-111. doi: 10.1037/e633982013-391.
    [Google Scholar]
  35. Stahl, S. A., Jacobson, M. G., Davis, C. E., & Davis, R. L. (1989). Prior knowledge and difficult vocabulary in the comprehension of unfamiliar text. Reading Research Quarterly, 24(1), 27-43. doi: 10.2307/748009.
    [Google Scholar]
  36. Stichting Lezen en Schrijven (z.d.). Eenvoudige taal voor laaggeletterden. https://www.lezenenschrijven.nl/wat-doen-wij/oplossing-voor-je-vraagstuk/factsheeteenvoudige-taal
http://instance.metastore.ingenta.com/content/journals/10.5117/TVT2022.2.002.MAAT
Loading
/content/journals/10.5117/TVT2022.2.002.MAAT
Loading

Data & Media loading...

This is a required field
Please enter a valid email address
Approval was a Success
Invalid data
An Error Occurred
Approval was partially successful, following selected items could not be processed due to error