The contribution of prosody to intelligibility, comprehensibility and accentedness in foreign language acquisition | Amsterdam University Press Journals Online
2004
Volume 26, Issue 2
  • ISSN: 1384-5845
  • E-ISSN: 2352-1171

Abstract

Abstract

The purpose of this paper is to review recent research illustrating the importance of prosody in foreign language acquisition, and to examine whether music might help in this learning process. The paper starts off by defining prosody and by examining previous research on foreign language prosody acquisition, stating the difficulties for learners and the potential effect of non-native prosody on communication, notably on comprehensibility, intelligibility and accentedness. A subsequent section focuses on prosodic characteristics of Dutch and the problems foreign language learners may encounter in acquiring them. Based on this general description of foreign language prosody acquisition, the paper then zooms in on the link between music and prosody, and on the potential effect of musical training, musical abilities or the use of music in the foreign language classroom on foreign language prosody acquisition. The paper ends with a short discussion on avenues for future research.

Loading

Article metrics loading...

/content/journals/10.5117/NEDTAA2021.2.006.DEGR
2021-10-01
2024-04-25
Loading full text...

Full text loading...

/deliver/fulltext/13845845/26/2/NEDTAA2021.2.006.DEGR.html?itemId=/content/journals/10.5117/NEDTAA2021.2.006.DEGR&mimeType=html&fmt=ahah

References

  1. Alexander, J., P.Wong & A.Bradlow (2005). Lexical tone perception in musicians and non-musicians. In: Proceedings of Interspeech 2005. Lisbon, Portugal.
    [Google Scholar]
  2. Altmann, H. (2006). The perception and production of second language stress: a cross-linguistic experimental study. Unpublished PhD dissertation University of Delaware, Newark, DE.
    [Google Scholar]
  3. Anderson-Hsieh, J., R.Johnson & K.Koehler (1992). The Relationship Between Native Speaker Judgments of Non-native Pronunciation and Deviance in Segmentals, Prosody, and Syllable Structure. Language Learning42, 529-555.
    [Google Scholar]
  4. Astésano, C. (2016). Prosodic characteristics of Reference French. In: S.Detey, J.Durand, B.Laks & C.Lyche (eds.), Varieties of spoken French: a source book. Oxford: Oxford Univeristy Press, 68-85.
    [Google Scholar]
  5. Bentley, A. (1966). Measures of music abilities. Berkshire: NFER-Nelson Publishing Company.
    [Google Scholar]
  6. Besson, M. & F.Faita (1995). Event-related potential (ERP) study of musical expectancy – Comparison of musicians with nonmusicians. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance21, 1278-1296.
    [Google Scholar]
  7. Boula de Mareüil, P. & Vieru-Dimulescu, B. (2006). The contribution of prosody to the perception of foreign accent. Phonetica63, 247-267.
    [Google Scholar]
  8. Caspers, J. (2010). The influence of erroneous stress position and segmental errors on intelligibility, comprehensibility and foreign accent in Dutch as a second language. In: J.van Kampen & RickNouwen (eds.), Linguistics in the Netherlands. Amsterdam: Benjamins, 17-29.
    [Google Scholar]
  9. Caspers, J. & K.Horłoza (2012). Intelligibility of non-natively produced Dutch words: interaction between segmental and suprasegmental errors. Phonetica69, 94-107.
    [Google Scholar]
  10. Chobert, J. & M.Besson (2013). Musical expertise and second language learning. Brain Sciences3, 923-940.
    [Google Scholar]
  11. Christiner, M. & S.M.Reiterer (2015). A Mozart is not a Pavarotti: Singers outperform instrumentalists on foreign accent imitation. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience9, 482.
    [Google Scholar]
  12. Chun, D. M. (2002). Discourse Intonation in L2: From theory and research to practice. Amsterdam: Benjamins.
    [Google Scholar]
  13. Clark, J. & C.Yallop (1995).An introduction to phonetics and phonology – Second edition. Cambridge: Blackwell.
    [Google Scholar]
  14. Coumel, M., M.Christiner & S.M.Reiterer (2019). Second language accent faking ability depends on musical abilities, not on working memory. Frontiers in Psychology10, 257.
    [Google Scholar]
  15. Crystal, D. (2008). A dictionary of linguistics and phonetics. Oxford: Wiley-Blackwell.
    [Google Scholar]
  16. Cutler, A. (2012). Native listening: Language experience and the recognition of spoken words. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  17. Cutler, A., R.Wales, N.Cooper & J.Janssen (2007). Dutch listeners’ use of suprasegmental cues to English stress. Proceedings of the 16th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences, Saarbrücken, 1913-1916.
    [Google Scholar]
  18. Cutler, A. (2015). Lexical stress in English pronunciation. In: M.Reed & J. M.Levis (eds.), The Handbook of English Pronunciation. Chichester: Wiley, 106-124.
    [Google Scholar]
  19. d’Alessandro, C. (2010). Analyse des différents stimuli auditifs : musique, langage et bruit. In: B.Lechevalier, H.Platel & F.Eustache (eds.), Le cerveau musicien. Bruxelles: De Boeck Supérieur, 33-46.
    [Google Scholar]
  20. Dankovicová, J., J.House, A.Crooks & K.Jones (2016). The relationship between musical skills, music training, and intonation analysis skills. Language and Speech50, 177-225.
    [Google Scholar]
  21. Deen, J., C.Van Veen & L.Schutte (2014). Taalriedels. Amsterdam: Boom.
    [Google Scholar]
  22. Degrave, P. (2019). Music in the Foreign Language Classroom: How and Why?Journal of Language Teaching and Research10, 412-420.
    [Google Scholar]
  23. Degrave, P. (2020). Music training and the use of songs or rhythm: do they help for lexical stress perception?IRAL: International Review of Applied Linguistics in Language Teaching (in press).
    [Google Scholar]
  24. Delogu, F., G.Lampis & M.Olivetti Belardinelli (2006). Music-to-language transfer effect: may melodic ability improve learning of tonal languages by native nontonal speakers?Cognitive Processing7, 203-207.
    [Google Scholar]
  25. Di Cristo, A. (2013). La prosodie de la parole. Bruxelles: De Boeck.
    [Google Scholar]
  26. Dupoux, E., N.Sebastián-Gallés, E.Navarrete & S.Peperkamp (2008). Persistent stress ‘deafness’: the case of French learners of Spanish. Cognition106, 682-706.
    [Google Scholar]
  27. Ezquerra, Á. O. (2012). Relación entre la aptitud musical y el grado de comprensibilidad del habla en una segunda lengua: estudio de un grupo de alumnos de español de la escuela secundaria inglesa. Porta Linguarum17, 223-236.
    [Google Scholar]
  28. Field, J. (2005). Intelligibility and the listener: the role of lexical stress. TESOL Quarterly39, 399-423.
    [Google Scholar]
  29. Fomina, A. (2000). Song Melody Influence on Speech Intonation Memorization. In: C.Woods, G.B.Luck, R.Brochard, S.A.O’Neill & J.A.Sloboda (eds.) Proceedings of the Sixth International Conference on Music Perception and Cognition. Keele: Department of Psychology, CD-ROM.
    [Google Scholar]
  30. Francois, C. & D.Schön (2011). Musical expertise boosts implicit learning of both musical and linguistic structures. Cerebral Cortex21, 2357-2365.
    [Google Scholar]
  31. Gaser, C. & G.Schlaug (2003). Brain structures differ between musicians and non-musicians. Journal of Neuroscience23, 9240-9245.
    [Google Scholar]
  32. Gordon, E.E. (1989). Advanced Measures of music audiation. Chicago: GIA.
    [Google Scholar]
  33. Gosselin, N., I.Peretz, J.Clément & S.D.Bella (2010). Comment le cerveau reconnaît-il la musique? In: B.Lechevalier, H.Platel & F.Eustache (eds.), Le cerveau musicien. Bruxelles: De Boeck Supérieur, 91-122.
    [Google Scholar]
  34. Gottfried, T. (2007). Music and language learning: Effect of musical training on learning L2 speech contrasts. In: O.Bohn & M.Munro, Language Experience in Second Language Speech Learning: In honor of James Emil Flege. Philadelphia: Benjamins, 221-237.
    [Google Scholar]
  35. Gottfried, T. L., A.M.Staby & C.J.Ziemer (2004). Musical experience and Mandarin tone discrimination and imitation. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America115, 2545-2545.
    [Google Scholar]
  36. Graham, C. (1993). Grammarchants: Student Book. New-York: Oxford University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  37. Gralinska-Brawata, A. & P.Rybinska (2017). The relationship between the production of word stress and musical abilities in Polish learners of English. Research in Language15, 265-283.
    [Google Scholar]
  38. Gussenhoven, C. & A.Chen, A. (red.). (2020). The Oxford Handbook of Language Prosody. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  39. Hao, Y.C. (2012). Second language acquisition of Mandarin Chinese tones by tonal and non-tonal language speakers. Journal of Phonetics40, 269-279.
    [Google Scholar]
  40. Heuven, V. J. van, J.G.Kruyt & J.W.de Vries (1981). Buitenlandsheid en begrijpelijkheid in het Nederlands van buitenlandse arbeiders, een verkennende studie. Forum der Letteren22, 170-178.
    [Google Scholar]
  41. Heuven, V. J. van (2018). Acoustic correlates and perceptual cues of word and sentence stress – Towards a cross-linguistic perspective. In: H.van der Hulst, J.Heinz & R.Goedemans (eds.), The Study of Word Stress and Accent: Theories, methods and data. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 15-59.
    [Google Scholar]
  42. Hiligsmann, P. & S.Heiderscheidt (2000). De accentuering in de tussentaal van Franstalige leerders van het Nederlands. Leuvense Bijdragen89, 117.
    [Google Scholar]
  43. Hiligsmann, P., L.Rasier (2007). Uitspraakleer Nederlands voor Franstaligen. Waterloo: Plantyn.
    [Google Scholar]
  44. Huang, B. H. & S.A.Jun (2011). The Effect of Age on the Acquisition of Second Language Prosody. Language and Speech54, 387-414.
    [Google Scholar]
  45. Intartaglia, B., T.White-Schwoch, N.Kraus & D.Schön (2017). Music training enhances the automatic neural processing of foreign speech sounds. Scientific Reports7, 1-7.
    [Google Scholar]
  46. Isaacs, T. & P.Trofimovich (2011). Phonological memory, attention control, and musical ability: Effects of individual differences on rater judgments of second language speech. Applied Psycholinguistics32, 113-140.
    [Google Scholar]
  47. Jackendoff, R. (2009). Parallels and nonparallels between language and music. Music perception: an interdisciplinary journal26, 195-204.
    [Google Scholar]
  48. Jentschke, S. & S.Koelsch (2009). Musical training modulates the development of syntax processing in children. NeuroImage47, 735-744.
    [Google Scholar]
  49. Kolinsky, R., H.Cuvelier, V.Goetry, I.Peretz & J.Morais (2009). Music Training Facilitates Lexical Stress Processing. Music Perception: An Interdisciplinary Journal26, 235-246.
    [Google Scholar]
  50. Kolly, M.-J., P.Boula de Mareüil, A.Leemann & V.Dellwo (2017). Listeners use temporal information to identify French- and English-accented speech. Speech Communication86, 121-134.
    [Google Scholar]
  51. Kooij, J. & M.van Oostendorp (2003). Fonologie. Uitnodiging tot de klankleer van het Nederlands. Amsterdam: Amsterdam University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  52. Kraus, N. & B.Chandrasekaran (2010). Music training for the development of auditory skills. Nature Reviews Neuroscience11, 599.
    [Google Scholar]
  53. Lee, C.Y. & T.H.Hung (2008). Identification of Mandarin tones by English-speaking musicians and nonmusicians. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America124, 3235-3248.
    [Google Scholar]
  54. Li, A. & B.Post (2014). L2 acquisition of prosodic properties of speech rhythm: evidence from L1 Mandarin and German learners of English. Studies in Second Language Acquisition36, 223-255.
    [Google Scholar]
  55. Maastricht, L. van, T.Zee, E.Krahmer & M.Swerts (2017). L1 perceptions of L2 prosody: The interplay between intonation, rhythm, and speech rate and their contribution to accentedness and comprehensibility. Proceedings of Interspeech 2017, 364-368.
    [Google Scholar]
  56. Maastricht, L van., T.Zee, E.Krahmer & M.Swerts (2020). The interplay of prosodic cues in the L2: How intonation, rhythm, and speech rate in speech by Spanish learners of Dutch contribute to L1 Dutch perceptions of accentedness and comprehensibility. Speech Communication, in press.
    [Google Scholar]
  57. Maess, B., S.Koelsch, T.C.Gunter & A.D.Friederici (2001). Musical syntax is processed in Broca’s area: an MEG study. Nature Neuroscience4, 540-545.
    [Google Scholar]
  58. Magne, C., D.Schön & M.Besson (2006). Musician children detect pitch violations in both music and language better than nonmusician children: behavioral and electrophysiological approaches. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience18, 199-211.
    [Google Scholar]
  59. Marie, C., F.Delogu, G.Lampis, M.O.Belardinelli & M.Besson (2011). Influence of Musical Expertise on Segmental and Tonal Processing in Mandarin Chinese. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience23, 2701-2715.
    [Google Scholar]
  60. Marques, C., S.Moreno, S.L.Castro & M.Besson (2007). Musicians detect pitch violation in a foreign language better than nonmusicians: behavioral and electrophysiological evidence. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience19, 1453-1463.
    [Google Scholar]
  61. Mishan, F. (2005). Designing authenticity into language learning materials. Bristol: Intellect.
    [Google Scholar]
  62. Moreno, S. & G.M.Bidelman (2014). Examining neural plasticity and cognitive benefit through the unique lens of musical training. Hearing Research308, 84-97.
    [Google Scholar]
  63. Mosing, M. A., G.Madison, N.L.PedersenF.Ullén (2016). Investigating cognitive transfer within the framework of music practice: genetic pleiotropy rather than causality. Developmental Science19, 504-512.
    [Google Scholar]
  64. Munro, M. J. (1995). Nonsegmental Factors In Foreign Accent: Ratings of Filtered Speech. Studies in Second Language Acquisition17, 17-34.
    [Google Scholar]
  65. Munro, M. J. & T.M.Derwing (2001). Modeling Perceptions of the Accentedness and Comprehensibility of L2 Speech. The Role of Speaking Rate. Studies in second language acquisition23, 451-468.
    [Google Scholar]
  66. Pantev, C., R.Oostenveld, A.Engelien, B.Ross, L.E.Roberts & M.Hoke (1998). Increased auditory cortical representation in musicians. Nature392, 811-814.
    [Google Scholar]
  67. Pastuszek-LipińskaB. (2008) Musicians Outperform Nonmusicians in Speech Imitation. In: R.Kronland-Martinet, S.Ystad & K.Jensen (eds.), Computer Music Modeling and Retrieval. Sense of Sounds. CMMR 2007. Lecture Notes in Computer Science 4969. Berlin: Springer, 56-73.
    [Google Scholar]
  68. Patel, A. D., E.Gibson, J.Ratner, M.Besson & P.J.Holcomb (1998). Processing Syntactic Relations in Language and Music: An Event-Related Potential Study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience10, 717-733.
    [Google Scholar]
  69. Patel, A. D. (2003). Language, music, syntax and the brain. Nature Neuroscience6, 674-681.
    [Google Scholar]
  70. Patel, A. D. (2003). Rhythm in language and music: parallels and differences. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences999, 140-143.
    [Google Scholar]
  71. Patel, A. D. (2010). Music, language, and the brain. Oxford University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  72. Patel, A. D. (2013). Sharing and Nonsharing of Brain Resources for Language and Music. In: M.Arbib (eds.), Language, music, and the brain: A mysterious relationship. Cambridge MA: MIT Press, 329-355.
    [Google Scholar]
  73. Peperkamp, S. & Dupoux, E. (2002). A typological study of stress ‘deafness’. In: C.Gussenhoven & N.Warner (eds.), Laboratory Phonology 7. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter, 203-240.
    [Google Scholar]
  74. Peretz, I. (2006). The nature of music from a biological perspective. Cognition100, 1-32.
    [Google Scholar]
  75. Peretz, I. (2009). Music, Language and Modularity Framed in Action. Psychologica Belgica49, 157-175.
    [Google Scholar]
  76. Peretz, I., D.Vuvan, M.E.Lagrois & J.L.Armony (2015). Neural overlap in processing music and speech. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences370, 20140090.
    [Google Scholar]
  77. Rasier, L. (2006). Prosodie en vreemdetaalverwerving. Accentdistributie in het Frans en het Nederlands als vreemde taal. Unpublished PhD dissertation Université catholique de Louvain, Louvain-la-Neuve.
    [Google Scholar]
  78. Rasier, L. & P.Hiligsmann (2007). Prosodic transfer from L1 to L2. Theoretical and methodological issues. Cahiers de Linguistique Française28, 41.
    [Google Scholar]
  79. Rietveld, A.C.M. & V.J.van Heuven (2016). Algemene Fonetiek (4th edition). Bussum: Coutinho.
    [Google Scholar]
  80. Schellenberg, E. G. & I.Peretz (2008). Music, language and cognition: unresolved issues. Trends in Cognitive Sciences12, 45-46.
    [Google Scholar]
  81. Schön, D., M.Boyer, S.Moreno, M.Besson, I.Peretz & R.Kolinsky (2008). Songs as an aid for language acquisition. Cognition106, 975-983.
    [Google Scholar]
  82. Schön, D., C.Magne & M.Besson (2004). The music of speech: music training facilitates pitch processing in both music and language. Psychophysiology41, 341-349.
    [Google Scholar]
  83. Sereno, J., L.Lammers & A.Jongmann (2016). The relative contribution of segments and intonation to the perception of foreign-accented speech. Applied psycholinguistics37, 303-322.
    [Google Scholar]
  84. Sluijter, A. M. & V.J.van Heuven (1996). Acoustic correlates of linguistic stress and accent in Dutch and American English. In: A.M.Sluijter & V.J.van Heuven (eds.), Proceedings of the International Congress of Spoken Language Processing. Philadelphia, 630-633.
    [Google Scholar]
  85. Thompson, W.F., E.G.Schellenberg & G.Husain (2003). Perceiving prosody in speech. Effects of music lessons. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences999, 530-532.
    [Google Scholar]
  86. Tremblay, A. (2009). Phonetic variability and the variable perception of L2 word stress by French Canadian listeners. International Journal of Bilingualism13, 35-62.
    [Google Scholar]
  87. Trofimovich, P. & W.Baker (2006). Learning Second Language Suprasegmentals: Effect of L2 Experience on Prosody and Fluency Characteristics of L2 Speech. Studies in Second Language Acquisition28, 1-30.
    [Google Scholar]
  88. van Els, T. & K.de Bot (1987). The Role of Intonation in Foreign Accent. The Modern Language Journal71, 147-155.
    [Google Scholar]
  89. Verboog, M. & L.Ader (2016). Taalraps, een aanstekelijke manier om spreektaal te oefenen. Amsterdam: Uitgeverij Boom.
    [Google Scholar]
  90. Wartenburger, I., J.Steinbrink, S.Telkemeyer, M.Friedrich, A.D.Friederici & H.Obrig (2007). The processing of prosody: Evidence of interhemispheric specialization at the age of four. NeuroImage34, 416-425.
    [Google Scholar]
  91. Winters, S. & M.G.O’Brien (2013). Perceived accentedness and intelligibility: The relative contributions of F0 and duration. Speech communication55, 486-507.
    [Google Scholar]
  92. Wolfe, D. E. & L.K.Noguchi (2009). The use of music with young children to improve sustained attention during a vigilance task in the presence of auditory distractions. Journal of Music Therapy46, 69-82.
    [Google Scholar]
  93. Wong, P. C. M., E.Skoe, N.M.Russo, T.Dees & N.Kraus (2007). Musical experience shapes human brainstem encoding of linguistic pitch patterns. Nature neuro- science10, 420-422.
    [Google Scholar]
  94. Yu, V. & J.E.Andruski (2010). A cross-language study of perception of lexical stress in English. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research39, 323-344.
    [Google Scholar]
  95. Zhang, Y., F.Baills & P.Prieto (2020). Hand-clapping to the rhythm of newly learned words improves L2 pronunciation: Evidence from training Chinese adolescents with French words. Language Teaching Research24, 666-689.
    [Google Scholar]
http://instance.metastore.ingenta.com/content/journals/10.5117/NEDTAA2021.2.006.DEGR
Loading
This is a required field
Please enter a valid email address
Approval was a Success
Invalid data
An Error Occurred
Approval was partially successful, following selected items could not be processed due to error